Cross-cultural determination could be a very perplexing field to understand with many different viewpoints, aims and concepts. The origins of cross-cultural determination in the 19th century world of colonialism was strongly grounded in the idea of cultural evolution, which claimed that all societies progress through an same series of sure evolutionary stages.
The origin of the word culture comes from the Latin verb colere = "tend, guard, cultivate, till". This idea is a human found rather than a stock of nature. The use of the English word in the sense of "cultivation through education" is first recorded in 1510. The use of the word to mean "the intellectual side of civilization" is from 1805; that of "collective customs and achievements of a people" is from 1867. The term Culture shock was first used in 1940.
How do we define culture?
There are really hundreds of different definitions as writers have attempted to provide the all-encompassing definition.
Culture consists of language, ideas, beliefs, customs, taboos, codes, institutions, tools, techniques, works of art, rituals, ceremonies and symbols. It has played a crucial role in human evolution, allowing human beings to adapt the environment to their own purposes rather than depend solely on natural selection to perform adaptive success. Every human society has its own singular culture, or sociocultural system. (Adapted from source: Encyclopaedia Britannica)
Generally culture can be seen as consisting of three elements:
- Values - Values are ideas that tell what in life is considered important.
- Norms - Norms consists of expectations of how citizen should behave in different situations.
- Artefacts - Things or material culture - reflects the culture's values and norms but are tangible and man-made by man.
Origins and evolution of Cross-cultural analysis
The first cross-cultural analyzes done in the West, were by anthropologists like Edward Burnett Tylor and Lewis H Morgan in the 19th century. Anthropology and collective Anthropology have come a long way since the belief in a gradual climb from stages of lower savagery to civilization, epitomized by Victorian England. Nowadays the idea of "culture" is in part a reaction against such earlier Western concepts and anthropologists argue that culture is "human nature," and that all citizen have a capacity to classify experiences, encode classifications symbolically and recapitulate such abstractions to others.
Typically anthropologists and collective scientists tend to study citizen and human behavior among exotic tribes and cultures living in far off places rather than do field work among white-collared literate adults in modern cities. Advances in communication and technology and socio-political changes started transforming the modern workplace yet there were no guidelines based on investigate to help citizen interact with other citizen from other cultures. To address this gap arose the discipline of cross-cultural determination or cross-cultural communication. The main theories of cross-cultural communication draw from the fields of anthropology, sociology, communication and science of mind and are based on value differences among cultures. Edward T. Hall, Geert Hofstede, Fons Trompenaars, Shalom Schwartz and Clifford Geertz are some of the major contributors in this field.
How the collective sciences study and analyze culture
Cultural anthropologists focus on symbolic culture whereas archaeologists focus on material and tangible culture. Sociobiologists study instinctive behavior in trying to explain the similarities, rather than the differences between cultures. They believe that human behavior cannot be satisfactorily explained entirely by 'cultural', 'environmental' or 'ethnic' factors. Some sociobiologists try to understand the many aspects of culture in the light of the idea of the meme, first introduced by Richard Dawkins in his 1976 book The Selfish Gene. Dawkins suggests the existence of units of culture - memes - approximately analogous to genes in evolutionary biology. Although this view has gained some popular currency, other anthropologists ordinarily reject it.
Different types of cross-cultural comparison methods
Nowadays there are many types of Cross-cultural comparisons. One recipe is comparison of case studies. Controlled comparison among variants of a tasteless derivation is other form of comparison. Typically anthropologists and other collective scientists favor the third type called Cross-cultural studies, which uses field data from many societies to examine the scope of human behaviour and to test hypotheses about human behavior and culture.
Controlled comparison examines similar characteristics of a few societies while cross-cultural studies uses a sufficiently large sample that statistical determination can be made to show relationships or lack of relationships between sure traits in question. The anthropological recipe of holocultural determination or worldwide cross-cultural determination is designed to test or found a proposition through the statistical determination of data on a sample of ten or more non literate societies from three or more geographical regions of the world. In this approach, cultural traits are taken out of the context of the whole culture and are compared with cultural traits in widely diverse cultures to determine patterns of regularities and differences within the broad base of the study.
Aims of cross-cultural analysis
Cross-cultural communication or inter cultural communication looks at how citizen from different cultural backgrounds try to communicate. It also tries to yield some guidelines, which help citizen from different cultures to better recapitulate with each other.
Culture has an interpretative function for the members of a group, which share that singular culture. Although all members of a group or society might share their culture, expressions of culture-resultant behaviour are modified by the individuals' personality, upbringing and life-experience to a needful degree. Cross-cultural determination aims at harnessing this utilitarian function of culture as a tool for addition human adaptation and enhancing communication.
Cross-cultural administration is seen as a discipline of international management focusing on cultural encounters, which aims to examine tools to deal with cultural differences seen as sources of conflict or miscommunication.
How laypersons see culture
It is a daunting challenge to carry the findings of investigate and field work and discuss cross-cultural issues in diverse contexts such as corporate culture, workplace culture and inter cultural competency as laypeople tend to use the word 'culture' to refer to something refined, artistic and exclusive to a sure group of "artists" who function in a detach sphere than lowly citizen in the workplace. Some typical allusions to culture:
Different models of cross-cultural analysis
Culture is the section in the newspaper where they recapitulate theatre, dance performances or write book reviews etc.Culture is what parents teach their kids and grandparents teach their grandchildren.
"You don't have any culture," is what citizen say to you when you put your feet on the table at lunchtime or spit in front of guests.
"They just have a different culture," citizen say about those whose behaviour they don't understand but have to tolerate.
There are many models of cross-cultural determination currently valid. The 'Iceberg' and the 'Onion' models are widely known. The popular 'Iceberg model' of culture industrialized by Selfridge and Sokolik, 1975 and W.L. French and C.H. Bell in 1979, identifies a graphic area consisting of behaviour or clothing or symbols and artifacts of some form and a level of values or an invisible level.
Trying to define as involved a phenomenon as culture with just two layers proved quite a challenge and the 'Onion' model arose. Geert Hofstede (1991) proposed a set of four layers, each of which includes the lower level or is a follow of the lower level. According to this view, 'culture' is like an onion that can be peeled, layer-by layer to recapitulate the content. Hofstede sees culture as "the collective programming of the mind which distinguishes the members of one group or kind of citizen from another."
Cross-cultural determination often plots 'dimensions' such as orientation to time, space, communication, competitiveness, power etc., as complimentary pairs of attributes and different cultures are positioned in a continuum between these.
Hofstede dimensions to distinguish between cultures
The five dimensions Hofstede uses to distinguish between national cultures are:
- Power distance, which measures the extent to which members of society accept how power is distributed unequally in that society.
- Individualism tells how citizen look after themselves and their immediate house only in discrepancy with Collectivism, where citizen belong to in-groups (families, clans or organizations) who look after them in transfer for loyalty.
- The dominant values of Masculinity, focussing on achievement and material success are contrasted with those of Femininity, which focus on caring for others and ability of life.
- Uncertainty avoidance measures the extent to which citizen feel threatened by uncertainty and ambiguity and try to avoid these situations.
- Confucian dynamism. This Long-term versus Short-term Orientation measured the fostering of virtues associated to the past, i.e., respect for tradition, significance of keeping face and thrift.
Trompenaars dimensions to distinguish between cultures
Trompenaars and Hampden-Turner (1997) adopt a similar onion-like model of culture. However, their model expands the core level of the very basic two-layered model, rather than the outer level. In their view, culture is made up of basic assumptions at the core level. These 'basic assumptions' are somewhat similar to 'values' in the Hofstede model.
Trompenaars and Charles Hampden-Turner use seven dimensions for their model of culture:
- Universalism vs Particularism (what is more leading - rules or relationships?)
- Individualism vs Communitarianism (do we function in a group or as an individual?)
- Neutral vs Emotional (do we display our emotions or keep them in check?)
- Specific vs Diffuse (how far do we get involved?)
- Achievement vs Ascription (do we have to prove ourselves to gain status or is it given to us just because we are a part of a structure?)
- Attitude to Time
- Past- / present- / future-orientatedness
- Sequential time vs Synchronic time(do we do things one at a time or any things at once?)
Internal vs External Orientation (do we aim to operate our environment or cooperate with it?)
Criticism of current models
One of the weaknesses of cross-cultural determination has been the inability to transcend the tendency to equalize culture with the idea of the nation state. A nation state is a political unit consisting of an autonomous state inhabited predominantly by a citizen sharing a tasteless culture, history, and language or languages. In real life, cultures do not have precise bodily boundaries and borders like nation states. Its expression and even core beliefs can assume many permutations and combinations as we move across distances.
There is some annotation in the field that this coming is out of phase with global enterprise today, with transnational clubs facing the challenges of the administration of global knowledge networks and multicultural scheme teams, interacting and collaborating across boundaries using new communication technologies.
Some writers like Nigel Holden (2001) advise an alternative approach, which acknowledges the growing complexity of inter- and intra-organizational connections and identities, and offers theoretical concepts to think about organizations and many cultures in a globalizing enterprise context.
In spite of all the shortcomings and criticisms faced by the Hofstede model, it is very much favoured by trainers and researchers. There are two reasons for this. Firstly, it is a splendid and easy to use tool to quantify cultural differences so that they can be discussed. Discussing and debating differences is after all the main recipe of training and learning. Secondly, Hofstede's investigate at Ibm was conducted in the workplace, so Hofstede tools brings cross-cultural determination closer to the enterprise side of the workplace, away from anthropology, which is a matter for universities.
Bibliography and recommend reading:
- Dawkins, Richard (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press
- French, W.L. And C.H. Bell (1979). Club development. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
- Hofstede, Geert "Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind", 1997
- Holden, Nigel 2001, Cross-Cultural Management: A Knowledge administration Perspective, Financial Times Management
No comments:
Post a Comment